一区二区三区日韩精品-日韩经典一区二区三区-五月激情综合丁香婷婷-欧美精品中文字幕专区

分享

雅思閱讀第112套P3-Striking the right note

 新用戶5354KZYV 2024-01-23 發(fā)布于山東

Is perfect pitch a rare talent possessed solely by the likes of Beethoven? Kathryn Brown discusses this much sought-after musical ability.

The uncanny, if sometimes distracting, ability to name a solitary note out of the blue, without any other notes for reference, is a prized musical talent – and a scientific mystery. Musicians with perfect pitch – or, as many researchers prefer to call it, absolute pitch – can often play pieces by ear, and many can transcribe music brilliantly. That’s because they perceive the position of a note in the musical stave – its pitch – as clearly as the fact that they heard it. Hearing and naming the pitch go hand in hand.

By contrast, most musicians follow not the notes, but the relationship between them. They may easily recognise two notes as being a certain number of tones apart, but could name the higher note as an E only if they are told the lower one is a C, for example. This is relative pitch. Useful, but much less mysterious.

For centuries, absolute pitch has been thought of as the preserve of the musical elite. Some estimates suggest that maybe fewer than 1 in 2,000 people possess it. But a growing number of studies, from speech experiments to brain scans, are now suggesting that a knack for absolute pitch may be far more common, and more varied, than previously thought. 'Absolute pitch is not an all or nothing feature,’ says Marvin, a music theorist at the University of Rochester in New York state. Some researchers even claim that we could all develop the skill, regardless of our musical talent. And their work may finally settle a decades-old debate about whether absolute pitch depends on melodious genes – or early music lessons.

Music psychologist Diana Deutsch at the University of California in San Diego is the leading voice. Last month at the Acoustical Society of America meeting in Columbus, Ohio, Deutsch reported a study that suggests we all have the potential to acquire absolute pitch – and that speakers of tone languages use it every day. A third of the world’s population – chiefly people in Asia and Africa – speak tone languages, in which a word’s meaning can vary depending on the pitch a speaker uses.

Deutsch and her colleagues asked seven native Vietnamese speakers and 15 native Mandarin speakers to read out lists of words on different days. The chosen words spanned a range of pitches, to force the speakers to raise and lower their voices considerably. By recording these recited lists and taking the average pitch for each whole word, the researchers compared the pitches used by each person to say each word on different days.

Both groups showed strikingly consistent pitch for any given word – often less than a quarter-tone difference between days. 'The similarity,’ Deutsch says, 'is mind-boggling.’ It’s also, she says, a real example of absolute pitch. As babies, the speakers learnt to associate certain pitches with meaningful words – just as a musician labels one tone A and another B – and they demonstrate this precise use of pitch regardless of whether or not they have had any musical training, she adds.

Deutsch isn’t the only researcher turning up everyday evidence of absolute pitch. At least three other experiments have found that people can launch into familiar songs at or very near the correct pitches. Some researchers have nicknamed this ability 'absolute memory’, and they say it pops up on other senses, too. Given studies like these, the real mystery is why we don’t all have absolute pitch, says cognitive psychologist Daniel Levitin of McGill University in Montreal.

Over the past decade, researchers have confirmed that absolute pitch often runs in families. Nelson Freimer of the University of California in San Francisco, for example, is just completing a study that he says strongly suggests the right genes help create this brand of musical genius. Freimer gave tone tests to people with absolute pitch and to their relatives. He also tested several hundred other people who had taken early music lessons. He found that relatives of people with absolute pitch were far more likely to develop the skill than people who simply had the music lessons. There is clearly a familial aggregation of absolute pitch,’ Freimer says.

Freimer says some children are probably genetically predisposed toward absolute pitch – and this innate inclination blossoms during childhood music lessons. Indeed, many researchers now point to this harmony of nature and nurture to explain why musicians with absolute pitch show different levels of the talent.

Indeed, researchers are finding more and more evidence suggesting music lessons are critical to the development of absolute pitch. In a survey of 2,700 students in American music conservatories and college programmes, New York University geneticist Peter Gregersen and his colleagues found that a whopping 32 per cent of the Asian students reported having absolute pitch, compared with just 7 per cent of non-Asian students. While that might suggest a genetic tendency towards absolute pitch in the Asian population, Gregersen says that the type and timing of music lessons probably explains much of the difference.

For one thing, those with absolute pitch started lessons, on average, when they were five years old, while those without absolute pitch started around the age of eight. Moreover, adds Gregersen, the type of music lessons favoured in Asia, and by many of the Asian families in his study, such as the Suzuki method, often focus on playing by ear and learning the names of musical notes, while those more commonly used in the US tend to emphasise learning scales in a relative pitch way. In Japanese pre-school music programmes, he says, children often have to listen to notes played on a piano and hold up a coloured flag to signal the pitch. 'There’s a distinct cultural difference,’ he says.

Deutsch predicts that further studies will reveal absolute pitch – in its imperfect, latent form – inside all of us. The Western emphasis on relative pitch simply obscures it, she contends. 'It’s very likely that scientists will end up concluding that we’re all born with the potential to acquire very fine-grained absolute pitch. It’s really just a matter of life getting in the way.’

SECTION 3: QUESTIONS 28-40

Questions 28-35

Complete the notes below using words from the box.

Write your answers in boxes 28-35 on your answer sheet.

NOTES

Research is being conducted into the mysterious musical 28 _________________ some people possess known as perfect pitch. Musicians with this talent are able to name and sing a 29 _________________ without reference to another and it is this that separates them from the majority who have only 30 _________________ pitch. The research aims to find out whether this skill is the product of genetic inheritance or early exposure to 31 _________________ or, as some researchers believe, a combination of both. One research team sought a link between perfect pitch and 32 _________________ languages in order to explain the high number of Asian speakers with perfect pitch. Speakers of Vietnamese and Mandarin were asked to recite 33 _________________on different occasions and the results were then compared in terms of 34 _________________. A separate study found that the approach to teaching music in many Asian 35 _________________ emphasised playing by ear whereas the US method was based on the relative pitch approach.

List of words

ability

music lessons

pitch

words

tone

relative

cultures

note

Questions 36-40

Reading Passage 3 contains a number of opinions provided by five different scientists. Match each opinion (Questions 36-40) with one of the scientists (A-E).

Write your answers in boxes 36-40 on your answer sheet.

A

Levitin

B

Deutsch

C

Gregersen

D

Marvin

E

Freimer

You may use any of the people A-Emore than once.

36 ____________ Absolute pitch is not a clear-cut issue.

37 ____________ Anyone can learn how to acquire perfect pitch.

38 ____________ It’s actually surprising that not everyone has absolute pitch.

39 ____________ The perfect pitch ability is genetic.

40 ____________ The important thing is the age at which music lessons are started.

    本站是提供個(gè)人知識(shí)管理的網(wǎng)絡(luò)存儲(chǔ)空間,所有內(nèi)容均由用戶發(fā)布,不代表本站觀點(diǎn)。請(qǐng)注意甄別內(nèi)容中的聯(lián)系方式、誘導(dǎo)購買等信息,謹(jǐn)防詐騙。如發(fā)現(xiàn)有害或侵權(quán)內(nèi)容,請(qǐng)點(diǎn)擊一鍵舉報(bào)。
    轉(zhuǎn)藏 分享 獻(xiàn)花(0

    0條評(píng)論

    發(fā)表

    請(qǐng)遵守用戶 評(píng)論公約

    類似文章 更多

    中文字幕乱码免费人妻av| 国产av精品高清一区二区三区| 欧美激情一区二区亚洲专区| 亚洲午夜av久久久精品| 国产又大又黄又粗的黄色| 亚洲第一区欧美日韩在线| 亚洲国产性生活高潮免费视频| 少妇肥臀一区二区三区| 午夜免费精品视频在线看| 国产精品欧美激情在线播放| 日韩精品福利在线观看| 一区二区三区免费公开| 国产精品偷拍一区二区| 欧美成人免费视频午夜色| 久久精品中文字幕人妻中文| 日本在线视频播放91| 国产又粗又猛又爽色噜噜| 热情的邻居在线中文字幕| 日韩av欧美中文字幕| 国产免费一区二区不卡| 欧美成人黄色一区二区三区| 超薄丝袜足一区二区三区| 欧美日韩视频中文字幕| 翘臀少妇成人一区二区| 高清免费在线不卡视频| 91精品视频免费播放| 99久久精品午夜一区二| 中文字幕人妻日本一区二区 | 少妇人妻无一区二区三区| 国产免费自拍黄片免费看| 婷婷色网视频在线播放| 欧美精品激情视频一区| 国产精品一区二区不卡中文| 久热久热精品视频在线观看| 国产又色又爽又黄又大| 日韩国产中文在线视频| 中文字幕亚洲精品人妻| 好吊日视频这里都是精品| 色欧美一区二区三区在线| 东京干男人都知道的天堂| 国产精品免费视频视频|